[http://www.sciencenews.org/view/generic/id/43531/title/Using_dead_stars_to_spot_gravitational_waves]
New technique could tune into cosmic symphony on the cheap
[http://www.sciencenews.org/view/generic/id/43519/title/Hobbit_foot%2C_hippo_skulls__deepen_ancestral_mystery]

Fossil studies add some new twists to debate about a tiny, humanlike species

Fossil hobbits followed a simple motto: Walk, don’t run. That’s the implication of a new analysis of foot bones from an 18,000-year-old partial skeleton assigned to the controversial species Homo floresiensis, or hobbits.

But the relatively long and unusually primitive left foot of the female skeleton, known as LB1, feeds into growing confusion about the evolutionary origins of hobbits, researchers report in the May 7 Nature. So does another new investigation, of fossil hippo skulls, in the same issue.

“The hobbit foot could have made something like the more than 3-million-year-old Laetoli footprints in Tanzania but probably not the modern-looking footprints ascribed to 1.5-million-year-old H. erectus in Kenya,” says anthropologist William Jungers of Stony Brook University Medical Center in New York (SN: 3/28/09, p. 14). Hobbits walked with a shuffling gait but would have had difficulty running, Jungers and his colleagues report.

LB1 stood at least a meter tall and displays primitive-looking skeletal traits from head to toe, Jungers says. H. floresiensis must have evolved from a species that was older, smaller and anatomically more primitive than Asian H. erectus, perhaps 2.4-million-year-old Homo habilis in Africa, Jungers proposes. An evolutionary transition from H. habilis to hobbits would have required little or no reduction in body size, in his view.

This proposal clashes with earlier interpretations of other hobbit fossils, also from the Indonesian island of Flores, that suggest a large-bodied H. erectus population from southeastern Asia evolved into H. floresiensis. H. erectus lived in southeastern Asia by 90,000 years ago, when hobbits first inhabited Flores. And other animals are known to have evolved into small species on islands, a process known as island dwarfing.

H. erectus could indeed have evolved a brain as small as that of LB1 via island dwarfing, assert Eleanor Weston and Adrian Lister, both paleontologists at the Natural History Museum in London. In support of that scenario, they report that two extinct dwarf species of hippopotamus from Madagascar possessed surprisingly small brains, based on a comparison with the modern-day hippo that evolved from the dwarf animals.

Weston and Lister studied fossil skulls of 36 pygmy hippos from species that died out about 6,000 years ago. Relative brain sizes of these creatures were as much as 30 percent smaller than would be expected by simply scaling down their larger evolutionary relatives.

These results, along with evidence of substantial brain-size reductions in island-dwarfed foxes (SN: 5/10/08, p. 7), suggest that H. erectus or another large-bodied ancestor could have evolved into hobbits, comments anthropologist Dean Falk of Florida State University in Tallahassee. That ancestral species contributed humanlike brain traits to H. floresiensis (SN: 4/25/09, p.9), Falk hypothesizes.

But island dwarfing in hobbits could not have produced so many primitive skeletal traits, Jungers responds. Consider that, relative to leg length, LB1’s foot is proportionally as long as that of Lucy, a 3.2-million-year-old species in the human evolutionary family. Other primitive foot features of LB1 include long, curved toes; a stubby, chimplike big toe; and no arch.

Scientists who regard LB1 as a modern human who suffered from a brain-stunting developmental disorder, and not a separate species, remain unconvinced. LB1’s foot looks long in Junger’s analysis only because the foot is being compared with an extremely short upper-leg bone, says evolutionary biologist Robert Eckhardt of Pennsylvania State University in University Park. An upper leg of that size — as well as many asymmetrically shaped head and lower-body bones — could have resulted from a developmental abnormality of some kind, Eckardt suggests.

Earlier comparisons of brain-size relationships in many mammal species, including island-living primates, indicated that LB1’s brain is too small to be that of an 18,000-year-old hominid, adds anthropologist Robert Martin of The Field Museum in Chicago. No data exist on relative brain size for a common ancestor of dwarf hippos and their mainland progeny, leaving Weston and Lister without a key comparison group, Martin notes.

The possibility that LB1’s many primitive skeletal features resulted from a developmental disorder “has not been seriously examined,” he says.

[http://www.sciencenews.org/view/generic/id/43434/title/Ants_may_be_the_Undead]

Living Argentine ant workers carry both a "dead" signal and an override "not dead yet" signal
Web edition : Monday, May 4th, 2009


UNDERTAKER
Ant nests have great undertaker capacity. Workers detect the dead and haul them outside to a pile of debris and corpses, often within an hour after death. Here a worker carries a young ant in the pupal stage to the refuse pile.Photo courtsey of the National Academy of Sciences and PNAS

Colonies of Argentine ants spreading around the world might be real-life invasions of the living dead.

Normal, still-breathing adult workers carry chemicals signaling “Dead ant — haul to burial pile” on their outer covering, proposes Dong-Hwan Choe of the University of California, Riverside.

What prevents awkward mistakes about who’s really dead are two additional compounds also found on the covering of living ants, Choe suggests. These compounds temporarily inhibit responses to the death cues by signaling, “Wait — still alive so far,” Choe and his colleagues report online May 4 in Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

Tests of ant reactions to chemicals applied to ant pupae provide the first experimental evidence for this “not-dead-yet” signaling system, Choe says.

Undertaker cues in ants have fascinated biologists studying the evolution of social living. “Being able to quickly remove dead individuals and other possible sources of pathogens and contaminants is extremely important for all animals living in societies, including us,” says Patrizia d’Ettorre of the University of Copenhagen. “Think about all the effort and money that we invest daily in waste management.”

Understanding life or death cues might also help in devising new ways to control Argentine ants, Choe says. The small ants have been moving into new territories worldwide and disrupting native ecosystems. He’s investigating ways to spread ant-killer from the dead ants to the colony mates that haul them away.

Earlier experiments had searched for a death cue that appeared on a corpse after death. Anesthetized ants typically get left alone until they come around, so stillness alone doesn’t trigger the undertakers. Yet a worker that really does die inside the nest gets hauled outside to a pile of mixed rubbish. But researchers haven’t yet agreed on what chemicals are involved and how they work.

Choe and his colleagues dropped fresh and not-so-fresh ant corpses into a lab colony and found that ants that had been dead for only an hour triggered about the same strong urge to remove them as ants that had been dead for 24 hours. Such a speedy response seems awfully fast for decomposition chemistry to trigger disposal, Choe says. So he and his colleagues experimented with the notion that, instead, some “still alive” cue fades fast.

To test different chemical signals, the researchers took advantage of the ants’ natural tendency to retrieve young ants at the pupal stage that somehow end up outside the nest. Pupae don’t have the same surface chemistry as adults, so researchers doused them with various signaling brews.

When treated with chemical extracts of ants that had been dead for an hour, 18 of 20 pupae were hauled to the refuse pile as if they were dead adults, suggesting that chemicals did play a role in the removal.

The team also treated the pupae with two compounds found on living ants and freshly dead ants but not found on ants dead an hour or more. These two compounds, dolichodial and iridomyrmecin, naturally dissipate quickly, the team found.

When doused with these compounds, the pupae were ignored. They were neither disposed of nor promptly brought back to the nest, suggesting other adults treated them as if they were indeed living adult workers, Choe and his colleagues say. Eventually, the workers did retrieve the pupae, as would be expected when an override signal — the dolichodial and iridomyrmecin — faded away, the researchers report.

Walter Tschinkel of Florida State University in Tallahassee says the idea is plausible but hasn’t been proven. “They did not put the last nail in that coffin,” Tschinkel says. Using pupae didn’t seem an appropriate test to him. He says he’d like to see what happens to dead adult ants treated with those compounds.

+ Recent posts